"Evolution built us to punish cheaters." That's how Judge Morris Hoffman began his brilliant book The Punisher's Brain: The Evolution of Judge and Jury (2014). In that book, Hoffman laid out the evidence and reasoning for the claim that social life and human civilization generally depend on our evolved instinct to punish ourselves and others when we or other people violate those social norms of cooperation that sustain any social order.
I have written about Hoffman's book in a previous post. I have suggested that what Hoffman says about the natural instinct for punishing those who disobey social norms corresponds to what John Locke identifies as the natural right of all people to punish those who transgress the law of nature, which is that "no one ought to harm another in his Life, Health, Liberty, or Possessions" (Second Treatise, 6). This Lockean law of nature corresponds to what Hoffman calls the three rules of right and wrong rooted in our evolved human nature to secure property and promises. Rule 1: Transfers of property must be voluntary. Rule 2: Promises must be kept. Rule 3: Serious violations of Rules 1 and 2 must be punished. Like Locke, Hoffman interprets "property" in a broad sense as starting with self-ownership and encompassing one's life, health, and possessions, as well as the life, health, and possessions of one's family and others to whom one is attached. Understood in this broad way, Rule 1 embraces criminal law and tort law, while Rule 2 embraces contract law.
Hoffman and Locke also agree in identifying three levels of natural punishment. Through self-punishment or first-party punishment, we punish ourselves through conscience and guilt. Through second-party punishment, we punish those who harm us by immediately retaliating against them or by later taking revenge against them. Through third-party punishment, we punish those who have harmed other people.
If Hoffman is right in claiming that biological evolution has built our brains to express this kind of punishment, then we should expect to see neural correlates for punishment at all three levels. Hoffman surveys the neuroscientific evidence for this.
In this post, I am beginning a series of posts reviewing this evidence, some of which was mentioned by Hoffman, but also some that has emerged over the past eight years.
Let's start with self-punishment. We punish ourselves by blaming ourselves for our misconduct, which is expressed through feelings of conscience and guilt. In feeling guilt, we blame ourselves for our past misconduct: we recognize that we have wrongly harmed others, and that they can rightly punish us. In feeling a conscience, we imagine blaming ourselves for some future misconduct, and this gnaw of conscience can motivate us to refrain from that misconduct.
From Charles Darwin to Edward Westermarck to Jonathan Haidt, evolutionary psychologists have explained these feelings of conscience and guilt as instinctive evolutionary adaptations for human beings as social animals who need to enforce the social norms of cooperation by punishing themselves for cheating. If this is true, then we should see evidence for these evolutionary adaptations in the human brain.
To search for such evidence, we need to somehow see the mind thinking in the brain. That became possible for the first time in the late 1970s with the invention of the positron camera and positron emission tomography (PET scan). Like magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), the PET scan depends on a fundamental postulate--"neurovascular coupling"--first proposed by neurologist Charles Sherrington in 1890: the most active parts of the brain will show an increase in blood flow in the vessels supplying them, because greater neural firing requires greater energy provided by the oxygen and glucose in the increased blood flow. This postulate was confirmed in the 1950s by the neurosurgeon Wilder Penfield: while operating on people with severe epilepsy, he would wake them up during the surgery, ask them to move their fingers, and he could see changes in color from an influx of blood to regions of the brain active in motor control. In the 1970s, neuroscientists David Ingvar and Niels Lassen developed a brain imaging method, by which a radioactive gas was injected into the carotid artery, so that a scintillation camera at the side of the subject's head could record the circulation of blood, which became the first functional imaging of the brain at work (Le Bihan 2015).
The PET scan also depends on radioactivity. Water that has been made radioactive in a cyclotron is injected in a vein of the arm. When the oxygen nucleus of water has been rendered radioactive, it ejects a positron (a positively charged electron) for a few minutes. When the radioactive water reaches the brain, the positron camera can record the higher quantity of positrons in those regions of the brain with increased blood flow.
In 2000, experimenters used PET scanning for the first neuroimaging study of guilt (Shin et al. 2000). They tested eight male participants for their experience of guilt. The participants were asked to write descriptions of two kinds of past personal events--one emotionally charged event that make them feel the most guilt they had ever experienced and two other events that created no deep emotion. These descriptions were then modified so that they were written in the second person and in the present tense. These scripts were read and tape-recorded in a neutral male voice for playback in the PET scanner. They were asked to listen carefully to the scripts and imagine the event as vividly as possible. After coming out of the scanner, they were asked to rate the intensity of their emotional states during the readings of the guilt and neutral scripts on a scale from 1 to 10.
Their average subjective rating for guilt was 8.8 for the guilt script and 0 for the neutral script. For shame, the average was 7.4 for the guilt script and 0 for the neutral script. For disgust, the average was 6.5 for the guilt script and 0 for the neutral script.
As compared with the neutral script, the PET scans showed increased blood flow to three areas of the paralimbic regions of the brain: the anterior (front) temporal poles, the anterior cingulate gyrus, and the anterior insular cortex/inferior frontal gyrus.
The paralimbic cortex surrounds the middle and lower parts of the brain's two hemispheres. It is a network of brain structures associated with emotional processing, goal setting, motivation, and self-control. This PET scanning study suggests that some of the neural circuitry in this paralimbic network supports the human experience of guilt by which we punish ourselves for violating social norms. And once we have learned how guilty we feel from our past misconduct, we will feel the pangs of conscience when we contemplate some similar misconduct in the future.
No comments:
Post a Comment